according to the text, what paradox is central to the study of personality development?

  • Journal List
  • J Stud Alcohol Drugs
  • PMC2696289

J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2009 Jul; 70(four): 489–498.

Parent, Family, and Neighborhood Effects on the Development of Child Substance Use and Other Psychopathology From Preschool to the Start of Adulthood*

Anne Buu, Ph.D., Cydney DiPiazza, B.A., Jing Wang, M.S., Leon I. Puttler, Ph.D., Hiram East. Fitzgerald, Ph.D., and Robert A. Zucker, Ph.D.

Received 2008 Aug 12; Revised 2009 Feb 20

Abstract

Objective:

Nosotros examined the long-term effects of childhood familial and neighborhood chance on adolescent substance utilize and psychiatric symptomatology.

Method:

This study used data from an ongoing ii-decade long study that recruited alcoholic and neighborhood control families through fathers' drunk-driving records and door-to-door canvassing in a iv county surface area. The sample included 220 male person, initially 3- to v-year-quondam children of the participant families, who received in-home assessments at baseline and thereafter at 3-yr intervals. Parental lifetime psychopathology and offspring symptomatology at ages 18-20 were assessed past semistructured diagnostic interviews. Census tract variables were used to indicate neighborhood characteristics.

Results:

The isomorphic parental symptomatology predicted offspring psychopathology. For marijuana-use disorder, major depressive disorder, and nicotine dependence, the other parental comorbidities were also significant predictors. Neighborhood residential instability in babyhood contributed to the development of late boyish booze-use disorder, marijuana-utilise disorder, major depressive disorder, antisocial personality disorder, and nicotine-dependence symptomatology. Although lower family unit socioeconomic condition in childhood contributed to more than adolescent marijuana-use disorder, major depressive disorder, and nicotine-dependence symptoms, neighborhood socioeconomic condition did not predict adolescent psychopathology. Longitudinal changes in neighborhood environments from early babyhood to adolescence had meaning furnishings on alcohol-use disorder, marijuana-use disorder, and major depressive disorder symptoms in late adolescence. A college frequency of family mobility from early childhood to boyhood predicted more nicotine-dependence symptoms in tardily adolescence.

Conclusions:

Findings indicate that parental psychopathology, family socioeconomic condition, and neighborhood residential instability are all important risk factors for the development of substance-use disorder and other comorbid psychopathology. Intervention programming might effectively utilize these early on parental psychopathology indicators to place risk and might target community activity to stabilize the social environment and provide youth services to counteract the effects of family transience.

Characterization of neighborhood effects on children'southward behavioral or mental health problems can provide important information for prevention programming as well as policy making. Existing studies addressing this result are mostly cantankerous-sectional (e.g., Winstanley et al., 2008) and have been reviewed comprehensively by Leventhal and Brooks-Gunn (2000). These cross-sectional studies are insufficient to establish a causal relationship between neighborhood characteristics and behavioral/mental health problems because they account for neither exposure fourth dimension nor individual differences in choice of a residence identify (Buu et al., 2007). Furthermore, without characterization of familial influence, models of more than distal neighborhood effects are at best incomplete and at worst provide inaccurate conclusions. Well-characterized prospective family unit studies are essential to sort out proximal and distal relationships and to institute an order of precedence of these furnishings.

Review of existing longitudinal studies

At that place are two types of longitudinal studies in the literature: quasi-experimental and observational studies. Each approach has its ain strengths and weaknesses. Residential mobility studies such as Moving to Opportunity (Katz et al., 2001; Kling et al., 2007; Leventhal and Brooks-Gunn, 2003) and the Yonkers project (Fauth et al., 2005) randomly assigned low-income families residing in high-poverty neighborhoods to relocate to low-poverty neighborhoods. Ii to 5 years later on (durations vary beyond studies), the children of movers reported fewer behavior bug than did the children of stayers. Gender differences were also institute in the Moving to Opportunity study, with female person youth benefiting most from the relocation (Kling et al., 2007). Although experimental studies (through random assignments) are theoretically better designs to control for misreckoning variables than observational studies, in reality, participants in these residential mobility studies tin can choose not to use the vouchers offered to them. For example, the compliance charge per unit was only 47% across all 5 sites of the Moving to Opportunity program (Leventhal and Brooks-Gunn, 2003). Although the compliance rate for the movers in the Yonkers project was 95%, the stayer group was non recruited through randomization, and pre-motility differences betwixt movers and stayers could not be tested because baseline data were unavailable (Fauth et al., 2005). Another practical issue with the quasi-experimental studies is that the mover sometimes experiences social alienation or hostility from new neighbors (Fauth et al., 2004; Rubinowitz and Rosenbaum, 2000). Such negative interaction or lack of interaction with new neighbors may confound the programme effects. In detail, older youth tended to experience more than discrimination distress (Fauth et al., 2005) and as well were able to travel back to their quondam high-poverty neighborhoods, thus vitiating the change-of-residence effects (Leventhal and Brooks-Gunn, 2003). To deal with these potential misreckoning effects, these studies usually control for family groundwork variables such equally ethnicity, age, and education during data analyses.

Conducted in natural settings, a modest group of longitudinal observational studies provide an culling way to examine neighborhood effects without problems from artificially manipulating participants' residences. However, neighborhood effects in these studies are evident simply when a number of relevant confounding variables, including family socioeconomic status, parental psychopathology, and family mobility, are controlled. Luthar and Cushing (1999) studied the effects of neighborhood socioeconomic status (indicated by census data) on internalizing/externalizing behaviors and substance use among the offspring (ages 7-17) of patients who met Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd Edition, Revised (DSM-III-R; American Psychiatric Clan, 1987), criteria for cocaine or opioid dependence. Neighborhood effects were shown only for drug use in their 2-year-long study. Hoffmann (2002) used data of 10th and twelfth graders from the National Educational Longitudinal Study to examine the impact of neighborhood environment (measured by zip code–level demography data) on youth drug utilise. Although no "neighborhood" effects were found, family mobility in the 2-year time frame contributed to drug employ. Although the study involved a national sample, the effect tested was not at the neighborhood level because zip code areas vary dramatically in size and may include residents with considerable heterogeneity of socioeconomic status (Thomas et al., 2006). Using self-report information, Lambert et al. (2004) constitute that perceptions of neighborhood disorganization in Grade 7 predicted increased tobacco, alcohol, and marijuana utilise in Class ix among urban black youths. A recent study on children of alcoholics and controls (Trim and Chassin, 2008) establish that, among children of nonalcoholics, higher neighborhood socioeconomic status predicted increased rates of alcohol utilize and consequences, whereas among children of alcoholics, the contrary clan was true. A significant effect for family mobility on adolescent alcohol use in the 3-yr interval was also found. Although the four studies reviewed above had primary interests in the straight impact of neighborhood on youth mental health, other longitudinal studies take examined if neighborhood contexts moderate other risk factors' effects. Roche et al. (2007) institute that the stakes of uninvolved and permissive parenting for problematic adolescent outcomes were greater in college chance neighborhoods based on two-wave data (over 16 months) from minority youth in depression-income urban areas. Analyzing iii-moving ridge data (over 6 years) of the Project on Human Development in Chicago Neighborhoods, Fauth et al. (2007) showed that participation in community-based clubs was positively associated with youth feet/depression in violent neighborhoods only, whereas church groups were protective against substance apply in irenic neighborhoods.

Unique features of the present study

The present study analyzed information from the 2d-generation participants in the Michigan Longitudinal Study (Zucker et al., 2000) to examine both familial and neighborhood furnishings over the course of childhood on the development of substance-use disorder and other psychopathologies at ages 18-20. At that place are several unique features to the study. Showtime, it extends the developmental period over which effects may occur to ages three through twenty, whereas prior longitudinal studies covered shorter developmental intervals (1-half-dozen years). Second, we examine not only neighborhood socioeconomic condition effects, every bit in prior longitudinal studies, only besides the effect of neighborhood residential instability—another factor identified as an important dimension of neighborhood influence in cross-exclusive studies (Leventhal and Brooks-Gunn, 2000). Third, the report uses DSM, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994), symptom counts on substance-utilise disorders and comorbid psychiatric disorders, including alcohol-use disorder, marijuana-utilise disorder, major depressive disorder, antisocial personality disorder, and nicotine dependence as outcome measures. Almost before piece of work used nonstandard parent or self-report assessments of mental health outcomes or substance use. Fourth, as psychopathology is transmissible from parents to children (Dierker et al., 1999), neighborhood furnishings are clearly evident but subsequently the effects of parental psychopathology and proximal family influences are taken into account, as done here. Relatedly, a significant short-term upshot of family mobility on substance use (ii-iii years) was found in 2 of the previous studies (Hoffmann, 2002; Trim and Chassin, 2008) but was not examined in other longitudinal work. This study examines the effects of both the frequency of family mobility and patterns of change in neighborhoods over the grade of 12 years on the evolution of these psychopathologies.

Theoretical framework and hypotheses

Figure 1 provides the theoretical framework that guided the statistical assay. It begins with the nigh proximal familial influences (genetic and socialization effects every bit indexed by parental psychopathology), the proximal opportunity structure (as measured by family socioeconomic condition), and so moves to the more distal neighborhood contextual influences. In the family-level model, nosotros tested familial manual of psychopathology based on the sequences listed in Table one. The conceptual framework guiding order of entry ever began with the parental psychopathology that paralleled the child psychopathology being predicted. Thereafter, the order of parental comorbidity was selected based on the strength of the comorbid human relationship betwixt the main and comorbid disorder, or the existing literature, which suggested a common genetic factor existed (eastward.g., booze-utilize disorder and antisocial personality disorder [Kendler et al., 2003], alcohol-use disorder and nicotine dependence [Jackson et al., 2000], major depressive disorder and nicotine dependence [Paperwalla et al., 2004]). Provisional on familial factors at baseline, we examined the impact of neighborhood factors at baseline. We hypothesized that neighborhood residential instability and neighborhood economic disadvantage during babyhood would both take negative influences on youth symptomatology in late adolescence. Given the baseline predictors, the effects of longitudinal changes in neighborhood environments on adolescent symptomatology were tested. Nosotros hypothesized that family mobility and worsening neighborhood environments both contributed to more symptomatology during late boyhood.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.  Object name is jsad489fig1.jpg

Theoretical framework for the impact of familial gamble factors and developmental changes in neighborhood environment on substance-employ disorders and other psychopathology outcomes in tardily adolescence. SES = socioeconomic status; AUD = alcohol-use disorder; MJUD = marijuana-use disorder; MDD = major depressive disorder; ASPD = antisocial personality disorder; ND = nicotine dependence.

Table 1

Theoretical sequences of the influences of parental psychopathology on the mental wellness outcomes of offspring

Step Offspring AUD Offspring MJUD Offspring MDD Offspring ASPD Offspring ND
Step 1 Parental AUD Parental MJUD Parental MDD Parental ASPD Parental ND
Pace 2 Parental ND Parental ND Parental AUD Parental AUD Parental MDD
Pace three Parental ASPD Parental AUD Parental ND Parental MJUD, MDD, & ND Parental AUD
Footstep 4 Parental MDD Parental ASPD Parental MJUD & ASPD Parental MJUD
Stride v Parental MDD Parental ASPD

Method

Design and sample

Alcoholic families were ascertained through men with drunk-driving convictions in a iv-county expanse involving a claret alcohol concentration of at to the lowest degree .xv% (if first confidence) or at least .12% (if a previous drinking-related legal problem had occurred). They also needed to meet diagnosis for probable/definite alcoholism and, because of offspring studies also conducted with this sample, were required to accept at least ane three- to 5-yr-old biological son. Only male children were initially recruited into the study, because sons of male person alcoholics are at highest hazard for subsequent alcoholism (Zucker et al., 1995). Although subsequent funding immune subsequently assessments to include a female person sibling, these girls joined the project at ages half dozen-11 and thus were not included in the analysis considering of bereft multiwave data. Recruitment criteria as well required the men to be living with the kid and his biological mother at the time of family consent. Alcoholic status of the mothers was gratuitous to vary. A contrast/control grouping of nonalcoholic families (neither parent with a history of substance use) was recruited through door-to-door canvassing in the same neighborhoods every bit the alcoholic families. This procedure also recruited an intermediate-risk grouping: families of parallel limerick who had alcoholic fathers without a history of alcohol-related legal or boozer-driving bug occurring during the life of their child. Original recruitment of fathers used Feighner criteria (Feighner et al., 1972). Every bit the report progressed, new diagnostic systems were introduced, and participants were rediagnosed using DSM-4 alcohol-use disorder criteria. A more detailed clarification of the study method is provided in an earlier written report (Zucker et al., 2000).

Michigan Longitudinal Study families received extensive in-dwelling house assessments at baseline (Time 1: ages 3-5) and, thereafter, at 3-year intervals (e.g., Time 2: ages six-eight; Time 5: ages 15-17). In this study, we included only the 220 male target children of the participant families (all white) who had completed Fourth dimension 6 (ages eighteen-20; i.e., who had outcome data available through belatedly adolescence and the offset of adulthood). The sample consists of 101 children (46%) recruited from courtroom alcoholic families, 46 (21%) from customs alcoholic families, and 73 (33%) from nonalcoholic families. 90-vii census tracts are represented. The average number of participants per census tract was 2, with a range of 1-10. All participants completed both Time ane and Time 6 assessments, although the completion rates from Fourth dimension 2 to Time 5 were 76%, 84%, 92%, and 95%, respectively. Missing assessments did not bear upon sample size because the assay required only information on frequency of family unit mobility and alter in neighborhood environment from Time 1 to Time 5.

Measures

Diagnostic Interview Schedule.

The Diagnostic Interview Schedule is a well-validated and widely used diagnostic musical instrument that gathers extensive information about psychiatric, physical, alcohol-related, and drug-related symptoms. The Diagnostic Interview Schedule-III (Robins et al., 1980) was administered at earlier waves, whereas the Diagnostic Interview Schedule-4 (Robins et al., 1996) was used for cess in later waves. The full numbers of symptoms corresponding to the DSM-IV criteria for some disorders are dissimilar betwixt the two versions. To make them comparable across waves, symptom counts from Diagnostic Interview Schedule-III data were prorated based on the Diagnostic Interview Schedule-Iv counts. The numbers of possible symptoms were 11 for alcohol-utilise disorder, x for marijuana-use disorder, 9 for major depressive disorder, seven for antisocial personality disorder, and 7 for nicotine dependence. Youth symptom counts on alcohol-use disorder, marijuana-use disorder, major depressive disorder, hating personality disorder, and nicotine dependence at Fourth dimension vi (ages eighteen-twenty) were used as mental health outcomes in tardily adolescence. Parental lifetime symptom counts on these disorders at baseline (when offspring were ages 3-five) were employed every bit the variables for parental psychopathology. Symptom counts, rather than diagnoses, were used because of their better statistical backdrop.

Family unit socioeconomic status.

At baseline, both biological parents filled out a demographic questionnaire that asked about his or her occupation. These data were coded for socioeconomic status based on the Duncan Socioeconomic Alphabetize, which was i of the best measures when our study began (Mueller and Parcels, 1981). The alphabetize is a continuous scale ranging from 0 (unemployed) to 9.04 (law professor). Family socioeconomic status was calculated by averaging the socioeconomic status coding of both parents.

Residential neighborhood characteristics.

Residential addresses of the 220 youth at Time 1 and Time 5 were matched with census tract coding at the census years (Apr 1 of 1980, 1990, or 2000) that were closest to the assessment dates. In this interval of approximately 12 years, the assessment dates of 87% of the participants represent to 1990 and 2000 censuses, 10% correspond to 1980 and 2000 censuses, and 3% correspond to 1980 and 1990 censuses. During this menstruum, 134 (61%) of the youth moved to different census tracts, making it probable that their neighborhood characteristics changed. For the participants who remained in the aforementioned census tract, census statistics for those neighborhoods also underwent change from Fourth dimension ane to Time 5 because of changes in local socioeconomic surround and resident composition. The following half-dozen neighborhood variables that may relate to individual substance use were computed to characterize neighborhood environments: (1) percentage of people living in different residence 5 years agone, (2) per centum of vacant households, (3) percentage of rented housing units, (four) adult unemployment rate, (5) poverty rate, and (six) per centum of people with education lower than high school diploma. Principal component assay on baseline information was conducted to investigate the possibility of reducing the dimensions. Results showed that in that location were two major components: (1) the neighborhood residential instability component, with high loadings on the first three neighborhood variables (explaining 49% of the variance), and (2) the neighborhood economic disadvantage component, with high loadings on the residuum of the three neighborhood variables (explaining 27% of the variance). To make the blended scores comparable from Fourth dimension 1 to Time v because the loadings in each moving ridge may vary, nosotros constructed each of the 2 composite scores past averaging the three corresponding standardized census variables. Nosotros used these two composite scores at baseline (ages 3-5) as measures for their neighborhood adventure factors in early babyhood. The longitudinal change in neighborhood environments from Fourth dimension 1 to Fourth dimension 5 (ages 3-17) on each of the composite scores was measured with the change score between these two fourth dimension points and categorized into three groups: (1) worse, (2) same, and (three) better, using the 25th and 75th percentiles of the difference scores as cutoff points.

Family mobility.

Family mobility frequency from Time ane to Time 5 was calculated past comparing each child's addresses over the interval. This captured family unit moves every 3 years prospectively, instead of relying on self-report retrospective data. The resulting variable ranged from 0 to iv.

Analytic approach

Because psychopathology outcomes are discrete count variables, linear regression models based on the normality assumption are not applicable. We therefore employed the Poisson regression model, the most mutual method for discrete count variables (Agresti, 2002; Long, 1997), to examine the effects of familial and neighborhood risk factors in childhood and longitudinal changes in neighborhood environment on psychopathology outcomes during late adolescence. Following the theoretical framework presented in Figure one, a Poisson regression model for each outcome was built through multiple stages. First, nosotros tested the effects of parental lifetime psychopathology using the theoretical sequence for each disorder listed in Tabular array 1. A particular parental lifetime symptom count stayed in the model if information technology had a significant effect (p < .05) higher up and across the effect of the parental psychopathology predictor(s) selected from previous step(s). After the submodel of parental psychopathology was congenital, it was tested to see if family socioeconomic status had an additional contribution (p < .05). If it did, it stayed in the final model; otherwise, it was excluded.

Given the influence of baseline familial factors, the impact of baseline neighborhood factors, including neighborhood residential instability and neighborhood economic disadvantage, was tested. These 2 variables stayed in the last model, regardless of their significance, considering the affect of longitudinal changes in neighborhood environment is evident only when the initial neighborhood environment is considered. Finally, controlling for the baseline risk factors, the affect of longitudinal changes in neighborhood environs was tested. This block contains three sets of variables: (1) frequency of family unit mobility, (two) longitudinal changes on neighborhood residential instability, and (3) longitudinal changes on neighborhood economic disadvantage. Both the 2nd and third sets contain ii binary variables to contrast (i) the group whose neighborhood became worse with the grouping whose neighborhood remained the same (i.e., the command group) and (ii) the grouping whose neighborhood became better with the control group.

Results

Descriptive statistics

Table 2 lists descriptive statistics for childhood and longitudinal predictors and tardily adolescent outcomes. Because most of the variables are neither continuous nor distributed symmetrically, the median was a better statistic for central tendency. Minimum and maximum values are listed to describe the overall spread of the distribution. 50 percent of the parents had at least ane lifetime DSM-Four symptom on all the comorbid substance use or psychiatric disorders except marijuana-use disorder (median = 0.5). Amongst them, nicotine dependence had the highest median (two.five). The variances amidst the symptomatology of all these disorders were large. Median family socioeconomic condition (ii.90) was lower than the median of the scale (4.52). Examples of the occupations corresponding to the median socioeconomic status are receptionist (2.90) and optician (2.92).

Tabular array 2

Descriptive statistics of the baseline/longitudinal predictors and the psychopathology outcomes in late adolescence (N = 220)

Variable Median (SD) Min. Max.
Baseline familial factors (ages three–5)
 Parental lifetime AUD symptom count 1.fifty (one.87) 0.00 seven.fifty
 Parental lifetime MJUD symptom count 0.50 (one.07) 0.00 4.50
 Parental lifetime MDD symptom count 1.00 (1.65) 0.00 7.l
 Parental lifetime ASPD symptom count one.00 (1.09) 0.00 4.00
 Parental lifetime ND symptom count two.fifty (two.24) 0.00 vii.00
 Family unit SES 2.90 (i.thirty) 0.80 7.26
Baseline neighborhood factors (ages three–5)
 Neighborhood residential instabilitya −0.07 (0.85) −1.58 3.46
 Neighborhood economical disadvantageb −0.28 (0.88) −1.xx 2.90
Longitudinal changes in neighborhood environments (ages 3–17)
 Frequency of family mobility ane.00 (0.xc) 0.00 4.00
 Change in neighborhood residential instabilityc 0.13 (0.89) −three.18 2.81
 Change in neighborhood economic disadvantagec 0.02 (0.97) −iii.51 three.50
Psychopathology outcomes in late adolescence (ages 18–20)
 AUD symptom count ane.00 (two.08) 0.00 10.00
 MJUD symptom count 0.00 (one.63) 0.00 7.00
 MDD symptom count 0.00 (2.60) 0.00 ix.00
 ASPD symptom count 1.00 (1.77) 0.00 seven.00
 ND symptom count 0.00 (one.21) 0.00 5.00

Neighborhood variables are average scores from standardized census variables; therefore, their magnitudes do not make intuitive sense. To describe the baseline neighborhood characteristics, the means (SD) of the 6 census variables used to calculate the composite scores are (one) 46% (10%) lived in a different residence five years agone, (2) iv% (2%) of households were vacant, (three) 30% (16%) of housing units were rented, (4) 7% (4%) of adults were unemployed, (five) xi% (nine%) were in poverty, and (half dozen) 19% (eight%) had no high schoolhouse diploma.

At least 50% of the children in the report had moved during the 12-year interval (Fourth dimension 1-Fourth dimension 5). The longitudinal changes in neighborhood residential instability and neighborhood economical disadvantage from Time 1 to Time 5 are both approximately normal distributions with ways around 0. We thus used the pinnacle and lesser 25% to ascertain the groups of children whose neighborhood environment changed significantly positively or negatively. Fifty percent of the children already had at least i DSM-Four symptom on alcohol-use disorder and antisocial personality disorder by ages xviii-20. As with their parents, there were large variances in the symptomatology of these youth.

Poisson regression models

Tabular array 3 shows the last Poisson regression models with regression coefficients, standard errors, and statistical testing results. Controlling for parental lifetime alcohol-utilise disorder symptomatology, neither the other parental comorbid disorders nor family socioeconomic condition had significant effects on youth booze-use disorder symptoms. In add-on to parental marijuana-employ disorder symptomatology, both parental nicotine dependence and family socioeconomic status contributed to youth marijuana-use disorder symptoms. For youth major depressive disorder symptoms, both parental antisocial personality disorder and family socioeconomic status had significant effects after decision-making for parallel parental psychopathology. Every bit with youth alcohol-apply disorder symptoms, the isomorphic parental psychopathology was the but pregnant familial risk gene for youth antisocial personality disorder symptoms. Controlling for the influence of parental nicotine dependence, parental major depressive disorder and family socioeconomic condition both had significant impact on youth nicotine-dependence symptoms.

Table three

Poisson regression of male target children's psychopathology outcomes in late adolescence (ages 18-20) on baseline familial/neighborhood factors and longitudinal changes in neighborhood environment (N = 220)

Variable AUD symptoms MJUD symptoms MDD symptoms ASPD symptoms ND symptoms
Intercept 0.34* −0.35* 0.06 0.17 −0.83*
(0.08) (0.10) (0.09) (0.10) (0.15)
Baseline familial factors (ages iii-5)
 Parental AUD 0.06*
(0.03)
 Parental MJUD 0.22* −0.11
(0.06) (0.06)
 Parental MDD 0.12* 0.07*
(0.03) (0.03)
 Parental ASPD 0.16* 0.xix*
(0.04) (0.05)
 Parental ND 0.06* 0.16*
(0.02) (0.04)
 Family SES −0.07* −0.04* −0.05*
(0.02) (0.02) (0.03)
Baseline neighborhood factors (ages iii-v)
 Neighborhood residential instability 0.17* 0.41* 0.thirty* 0.21* 0.24*
(0.06) (0.07) (0.06) (0.06) (0.09)
 Neighborhood economic disadvantage −0.10 −0.13 0.05 0.03 −0.01
(0.07) (0.08) (0.06) (0.06) (0.09)
Longitudinal changes in neighborhood environments (ages 3-17)
 Frequency of family unit mobility 0.11 0.12 −0.03 0.09 0.nineteen*
(0.06) (0.07) (0.07) (0.06) (0.08)
 Becoming more than stable −0.thirty* 0.05 0.04 −0.07 −0.01
(0.14) (0.16) (0.fourteen) (0.13) (0.18)
 Becoming less stable −0.04 −0.01 0.thirty* 0.05 −0.01
(0.12) (0.17) (0.xiii) (0.12) (0.18)
 Becoming more than affluent 0.fifteen −0.43* −0.06 −0.08 −0.25
(0.xiii) (0.20) (0.15) (0.14) (0.20)
 Becoming less affluent −0.04 0.07 −0.twoscore* −0.03 −0.05
(0.12) (0.fifteen) (0.15) (0.12) (0.17)

Baseline neighborhood residential instability had meaning effects on all outcomes; this was above and beyond the contribution of the familial chance factors. On the other hand, baseline neighborhood economic disadvantage did not contribute additionally to whatsoever of the adolescent symptoms. Decision-making for the effects of baseline chance factors and the patterns of longitudinal alter in the neighborhood, the frequency of family unit mobility had no upshot on any of the adolescent symptoms except for nicotine dependence. Children whose neighborhoods became more stable during the 12-year interval tended to take fewer booze-use disorder symptoms than those whose respective neighborhood environment stayed the same (i.eastward., the control), whereas children whose neighborhoods became less stable tended to have more than major depressive disorder symptoms than the control. Moreover, children whose neighborhoods became more than flush tended to develop fewer marijuana-use disorder symptoms than the command. The only unexpected event was that children whose neighborhoods became less affluent developed fewer major depressive disorder symptoms than the control.

Discussion

Parental psychopathology furnishings

As expected for all substance-use disorder and psychopathology outcomes, the isomorphic parental symptomatology predicted the level of offspring psychopathology. However, other parental comorbidities differed in their degree of influence. For nicotine dependence, our findings are consequent with a genetic link betwixt depression and smoking (Paperwalla et al., 2004; Quattrocki, 2000); after controlling for the result of parent lifetime nicotine-dependence symptoms, parent lifetime major depressive disorder symptoms still predicted boyish nicotine-dependence symptoms. Furthermore, parental lifetime nicotine-dependence symptoms predicted offspring development of non only isomorphic drug dependence but as well the development of symptomatology on the other drug with a smoking route of delivery (marijuana). In improver, parent lifetime hating personality disorder predicted offspring antisocial personality disorder as well as major depressive disorder symptoms. Disorder-specific transmission existed only for alcohol-use disorder and hating personality disorder; for the other three disorders, parental comorbid psychopathology had significant effects.

In that location are two long-term chance identification implications for these findings. First, to prevent adolescent marijuana-use disorder, major depressive disorder, and nicotine dependence, targeting only those children whose parents have parallel symptom histories will omit other significant hazard conveyed by parental comorbid psychiatric disorders. Second, because environmental run a risk factors from both the family unit and the neighborhood, in a higher place and beyond parental psychopathology, play a function in shaping these late-adolescent outcomes, an ecological approach to risk reduction that focuses on familial and neighborhood/community factors likewise every bit individual take a chance is warranted.

Neighborhood residential instability

Our study shows that living in an unstable neighborhood where residents move in/out ofttimes during babyhood is a significant correspondent to the evolution of all five adolescent psychopathology outcomes. According to Sampson et al. (1997), neighborhood residential instability hinders the formation of social cohesion among neighbors and weakens their willingness to intervene on behalf of the common good. The association between neighborhood residential instability and youth psychopathology may exist largely mediated by such depression "collective efficacy." Customs efforts to meliorate neighborhood environments for youth development, such equally providing support networks for families and building customs-level institutions to supervise and monitor the behavior of residents, particularly using age-appropriate youth activities, may usefully reduce the risk for youth mental disorders. The findings that children whose neighborhoods become more stable from early babyhood to boyhood tend to develop fewer alcohol-use disorder symptoms and children whose neighborhoods become less stable accept more than major depressive disorder symptoms shed some light on the potential of neighborhood intervention every bit a tool in the prevention armamentarium.

Neighborhood socioeconomic status versus family socioeconomic condition

Findings also evidence that, although lower family unit socioeconomic status during childhood contributed to more marijuana-use disorder, major depressive disorder, and nicotine-dependence symptoms in late adolescence, neighborhood socioeconomic status (disadvantage) during childhood was not a predictor for adolescent psychopathology. This is not a surprising result in light of other studies that plant family-level variables tended to exist more strongly associated with individual outcomes than neighborhood-level variables. The relative contributions of these 2 levels of influence on children's mental health were quantified in a nationwide study of 2-year-old twins (Caspi et al., 2000): Environmental factors shared by members of a family accounted for 20% of the population variation in children's behavior issues, whereas neighborhood deprivation deemed for only five% of this family-wide ecology event.

Although neighborhood socioeconomic status during early childhood did not contribute to adolescent outcomes, controlling for familial adventure factors, longitudinal changes in neighborhood economic surround over the form of childhood did. That is, a irresolute neighborhood environment during the 12-year interval of childhood and boyhood predicted the emergence of marijuana-apply disorder equally well equally major depressive disorder symptomatology in late boyhood. Nonetheless, the nature of this relationship was opposite across the two disorders, with an increase in neighborhood abundance predicting a lower level of marijuana symptoms and a decrease in neighborhood affluence predicting a lower level of major depressive disorder symptoms. The marijuana upshot is the more straightforward one, involving more difficult access to illegal drugs with increasing neighborhood abundance (Storr et al., 2004). The major depressive disorder upshot is not intuitively obvious, although an increasing literature has observed this clan (Luthar, 2003; Schwartz, 2004).

Family unit mobility

Previous studies accept observed a brusk-term effect of family mobility on adolescent substance utilize (Hoffmann, 2002; Trim and Chassin, 2008). We examined the long-term effect of family mobility after controlling for the patterns of longitudinal changes in neighborhood environment and other developmental risk factors and constitute that a higher frequency of family mobility predicted only a higher level of nicotine-dependence symptoms. Theoretical rationales for such an issue include heightened feelings of stress and feet, lessened parental supervision, and then on (DeWit, 1998). The nonsignificant effects of family unit mobility on the symptom levels of the other four disorders imply that information technology is where you move to, instead of how many times you move, that is going to make a departure.

Limitations and advantages

1 obvious limitation in our work is the absence of any measures of peer influence. Our concluding models did non include peer factors because nosotros chose to focus primarily on familial and neighborhood effects. Moreover, additional analysis did not find that participants' reports on both peers' substance utilise and delinquent beliefs at Time 5 (ages xv-17) predicted their own psychopathology outcomes at Time six (ages 18-twenty) to a higher place and across the familial and neighborhood risk factors already included in the model. Time to come studies might probe these complex interactions among familial, neighborhood, and peer influences in an ecological framework, just this was beyond the scope of the present piece of work.

Second, the study is observational rather than experimental; from that perspective, it is not the ideal design to plant causal inference. At the same time, it is virtually impossible to manipulate both the familial and neighborhood gamble factors. Notwithstanding, we advisedly controlled for important confounding factors in the regression model. Moreover, nosotros used risk factors measured over the course of childhood to predict psychiatric symptomatology in late boyhood. Thus, i may contend for the direction of causal human relationship based on the time series of events.

3rd, considering of the family unit report design, a homo recruited into the study had to reside with his son (ages 3-v) and the son's biological female parent at the time of initial recruitment. This recruitment criterion reduces external validity to some degree, considering results can be generalized only to children who were born in an initially coupled relationship and likewise simply to families at a relatively early on stage in the family life bicycle.

Fourth, our analyses focus on only the developmental psy-chopathology of male person children; therefore, the results cannot be generalized to girls. Because of the original recruitment protocol of the Michigan Longitudinal Study, only male target children had consummate childhood data. Time to come studies need to test similar models involving female person children from early babyhood to study the impact of familial and neighborhood risk factors on their symptom evolution. A final limitation is that the report relies on relatively macrolevel demography data to characterize neighborhood context; therefore, nosotros cannot evaluate hypotheses about the microprocesses through which neighborhoods may influence children. The social organizational characteristics of neighborhoods—established past aggregating responses of individuals to community surveys in the same neighborhood—would allow for a more than microlevel mechanistic test of the neighborhood influencing process (cf. Sampson et al., 1997).

This study too has two distinct advantages that differentiate information technology from well-nigh other studies in this area. One is the young developmental stage of the families at the time of recruitment; another is the extended interval of a 15-twelvemonth time bridge. This meant we were able to evaluate these familial and neighborhood effects across most the entire span of babyhood and adolescence rather than just a portion of that time. The narrow recruitment age range of the children too meant that these effects would not be diluted or confounded by the developmental heterogeneity of the sample at the time the contextual influences were operating.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Susan Refior and Wei Wang for their invaluable assistance with the initial information collection (SR) and in the training of this manuscript (WW).

Footnotes

*This enquiry was supported by National Establish on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism grant R37 AA-07065 to Robert A. Zucker and Hiram E. Fitzgerald.

References

  • Agresti A. Categorical Data Assay. second Edition. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III-R) Washington, DC: 1987. [Google Scholar]
  • American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) Washington, DC: 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • Buu A, Mansour G, Wang J, Refior SK, Fitzgerald HE, Zucker RA. Alcoholism furnishings on social migration and neighborhood effects on alcoholism over the grade of 12 years. Alcsm Clin. Exp. Res. 2007;31:1545–1551. [PMC complimentary article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Caspi A, Taylor A, Moffitt TE, Plomin R. Neighborhood deprivation affects children'southward mental health: Environmental risks identified in a genetic design. Psychol. Sci. 2000;11:338–342. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • DeWit DJ. Frequent babyhood geographic relocation: Its impact on drug use initiation and the development of alcohol and other drug-related problems amid adolescents and young adults. Addict. Behav. 1998;23:623–634. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Dierker LC, Merikangas KR, Szatmari P. Influence of parental cyclopedia for psychiatric disorders on psychopathology in offspring. J. Amer. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiat. 1999;38:280–288. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Fauth RC, Leventhal T, Brooks-Gunn J. Curt-term effects of moving from public housing in poor to heart-class neighborhoods on low-income, minority adults' outcomes. Social Sci. Med. 2004;59:2271–2284. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Fauth RC, Leventhal T, Brooks-Gunn J. Early impacts of moving from poor to centre-course neighborhoods on depression-income youth. J. Appl. Devel. Psychol. 2005;26:415–439. [Google Scholar]
  • Fauth RC, Roth JL, Brooks-Gunn J. Does the neighborhood context alter the link betwixt youth'south after-schoolhouse time activities and developmental outcomes? A multilevel analysis. Devel. Psychol. 2007;43:760–777. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Feighner JP, Robins E, Guze SB, Woodruff RA, Jr, Winokur G, Munoz R. Diagnostic criteria for use in psychiatric inquiry. Arch. Gen. Psychiat. 1972;26:57–63. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hoffmann JP. The community context of family construction and adolescent drug use. J. Marr. Fam. 2002;64:314–330. [Google Scholar]
  • Jackson KM, Sher KJ, Wood PK. Prospective analysis of comor-bidity: Tobacco and booze use disorders. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2000;109:679–694. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Katz LF, Kling JR, Liebman JB. Moving to opportunity in Boston: Early results of a randomized mobility experiment. Q. J. Econ. 2001;116:607–654. [Google Scholar]
  • Kendler KS, Prescott CA, Myers J, Neale MC. The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for common psychiatric and substance utilize disorders in men and women. Arch. Gen. Psychiat. 2003;lx:929–937. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Kling JR, Liebman JB, Katz LF. Experimental assay of neighborhood effects. Econometrica. 2007;75:83–119. [Google Scholar]
  • Lambert SF, Brownish TL, Phillips CM, Ialongo NS. The relationship between perceptions of neighborhood characteristics and substance use among urban African American adolescents. Amer. J. Commun. Psychol. 2004;34:205–218. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Leventhal T, Brooks-Gunn J. The neighborhoods they live in: The effects of neighborhood residence on child and boyish outcomes. Psychol. Bull. 2000;126:309–337. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Leventhal T, Brooks-Gunn J. Moving to opportunity: An experimental study of neighborhood effects on mental health. Amer. J. Publ. Hlth. 2003;93:1576–1582. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Long JS. Regression Models for Chiselled and Limited Dependent Variables. Thou Oaks, CA: Sage; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • Luthar SS. The culture of affluence: Psychological costs of material wealth. Child Devel. 2003;74:1581–1593. [PMC costless article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Luthar SS, Cushing 1000. Neighborhood influences and child evolution: A prospective written report of substance abusers' offspring. Devel. Psychopathol. 1999;xi:763–784. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Mueller CW, Parcel TL. Measures of socioeconomic status: Alternatives and recommendations. Kid Devel. 1981;52:13–thirty. [Google Scholar]
  • Paperwalla KN, Levin TT, Weiner J, Saravay SM. Smoking and depression. Med. Clin. No. Amer. 2004;88:1483–1494. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Quattrocki E, Baird A, Yurgelun-Todd D. Biological aspects of the link betwixt smoking and depression. Harvard Rev. Psychiat. 2000;eight:99–110. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Robins LN, Helzer JE, Croughan J, Ratcliff KS. The National Plant of Mental Wellness Diagnostic Interview Schedule: Its history, characteristics and validity. Arch. Gen. Psychiat. 1981;38:381–389. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Robins L, Marcus L, Reich W, Cunningham R, Gallagher T. NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule, Version Iv (DIS-4) St. Louis, MO: Washington University School of Medicine; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • Roche KM, Ensminger ME, Cherlin AJ. Variations in parenting and adolescent outcomes amidst African American and Latino families living in low-income, urban areas. J. Fam. Problems. 2007;28:882–909. [Google Scholar]
  • Rubinowitz LS, Rosenbaum JE. Crossing the Class and Color Lines: From Public Housing to White Suburbia. Chicago, IL: Univ. of Chicago Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • Sampson RJ, Raudenbush SW, Earls F. Neighborhoods and violent law-breaking:A multi level report of commonage efficacy. Science. 1997;277:918–924. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Schwartz B. The Paradox of Choice: Why More is Less. New York: Harper Collins; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • Storr CL, Chen C-Y, Anthony JC. "Diff opportunity": Neighborhood disadvantage and the chance to buy illegal drugs. J. Epidemiol. Commun. Hlth. 2004;58:231–237. [PMC costless article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Thomas AJ, Eberly LE, Smith GD, Neaton JD for the Multiple Risk Cistron Intervention Trial (MRFIT) Research Group. Zip-lawmaking-based versus tract-based income measures as long-term risk-adjusted mortality predictors. Amer. J. Epidemiol. 2006;164:586–590. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Trim RS, Chassin L. Neighborhood socioeconomic status effects on adolescent alcohol outcomes using growth models: Exploring the office of parental alcoholism. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs. 2008;69:639–648. [PMC free commodity] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Winstanley EL, Steinwachs DM, Ensminger ME, Latkin CA, Stitzer ML, Olsen Y. The association of self-reported neighborhood disorganization and social upper-case letter with adolescent alcohol and drug use, dependence, and access to treatment. Drug Booze Depend. 2008;92:173–182. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Zucker RA, Fitzgerald HE, Moses HD. Emergence of alcohol bug and the several alcoholisms: A developmental perspective on etiologic theory and life class trajectory. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen DJ, editors. Developmental Psychopathology, Vol. 2: Risk, Disorder, and Accommodation. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons; 1995. pp. 677–711. [Google Scholar]
  • Zucker RA, Fitzgerald HE, Refior SK, Puttler LI, Pallas DM, Ellis DA. The clinical and social ecology of childhood for children of alcoholics: Clarification of a study and implications for a differentiated social policy. In: Fitzgerald HE, Lester BM, Zuckerman BS, editors. Children of Addiction: Inquiry, Health, and Public Policy Issues. New York: Routledge Falmer; 2000. pp. 109–141. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Periodical of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs are provided here courtesy of Rutgers University. Center of Alcohol Studies


coaldrakekned1979.blogspot.com

Source: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2696289/

0 Response to "according to the text, what paradox is central to the study of personality development?"

Post a Comment

Iklan Atas Artikel

Iklan Tengah Artikel 1

Iklan Tengah Artikel 2

Iklan Bawah Artikel